TOEFL Experts Reading Practice 37

 
Reading Section
 
 
This section measures your ability to understand academic passages in English. You can skip questions and go back to them later as long as there is time remaining.
 
 
 
 
Now begin the Reading section.
 
Reading Section
 
 
This section measures your ability to understand academic passages in English. You can skip questions and go back to them later as long as there is time remaining.
 
 
 
 
Now begin the Reading section.
Signaling Theory
 
01-male-bird-shutting-up-female-bird

  There is a history of controversy within evolutionary biology over why honesty exists in animal communication. All animals, humans included, engage in communication that involves both a sender and a receiver, and this communication occurs both within and across species. The sender communicates a signal to a receiver, who then acts upon that signal. Male peacocks seek to signal their worthiness to females of the species by growing vibrant tail feathers, and gazelles engage in spectacular leaping displays, called stotting, upon seeing approaching predators in order to indicate their athletic prowess and therefore dissuade the predator from attacking.

  In some cases, a signal may be honest, meaning that the sender is conveying accurate information, but signals can also be dishonest when the sender is providing false information to a receiver. All signals, however, share the additional feature of being costly to produce and send. A peacock’s tail feathers have significant weight and take excessive energy to produce. A bird chirping to alert others in the flock of a nearby predator has now exposed its own location to that same predator, potentially making it more vulnerable to attack.

  Biologists have found that, in general, animals communicate with honest signals, but why? Deception would seem to provide short-term gains. For example, the male fiddler crab is known for its one large fighting claw, which it uses to compete for a mate with other male crabs. If a fiddler crab loses its claw in the fight, another claw that is lighter in weight and therefore less effective grows in its place. Although the crab can still scare off other mates with its new claw that is similar in size to the original, it is sending the dishonest signal to other males that it is strong and able to fight, although if challenged it would likely lose. Scientists theorize that signals must be honest on average, at least to a certain degree. If not, the intended receiver would eventually evolve to ignore the signal, rendering it useless. In the early 1970s, biologist Amotz Zahavi proposed the handicap principle: honesty is maintained through handicaps, or high-cost signals, which are naturally more believable.

  The handicap principle relies on the assumption that prominent signals of fighting ability or selection may be impossible, or impossibly costly, to fake. For example, a full-grown bull elk’s rack of antlers may weigh in excess of 40 pounds, a weight greater than a young, weak, or sickly individual could hope to carry. Therefore, an elk able to grow a large rack of antlers is honestly signaling its ability to defend itself in a fight. Male peacock tail feathers present the same honest signal to females about the male’s strength and desirability as a mate. The long tail not only takes significant effort to produce, but also creates issues for survival. The bright colors are more attractive to predators, and the length of the tail somewhat restricts flight and therefore the bird’s ability to escape pursuit. As with the elk, a weaker or sicklier bird could not afford to produce such ornamentation. In both examples, the signal of strength is, in actuality, a handicap to the animal sending it.

  Thus, these revealing handicaps are honest signals of strength in the sense that some members of the species are better equipped to handle the costs of these handicaps. While most biologists agree on the power of handicaps to maintain honest signaling, the question still debated is that of choice. Are handicaps the result of a genetic condition that allows only certain members of the species to express the handicap fully? Or do they happen when a more capable individual actively chooses to take on a visible hardship and therefore signal to others its underlying strength or abilities?

  The condition-dependent model suggests that the level of display of a behavior or trait is directly proportional to genetic quality and environmental conditions and therefore cannot be faked. Some point to bright, iridescent plumage of some bird species as a condition-dependent, and therefore honest, handicap. Without limited physical wear, low parasite load, and a nutritious diet, these shiny feathers are impossible to maintain. The choice model, alternatively, was advocated in 1990 by biologist Alan Grafen, who claimed that all animals have the option to display a large handicap, but each must choose whether and to what extent to display that handicap, according to its knowledge of its own ability level. The optional signal remains honest because low-quality individuals will never signal at a level that is higher than would be advantageous for them, because of the increased cost of those signals. 

Signaling Theory
 
01-male-bird-shutting-up-female-bird

  There is a history of controversy within evolutionary biology over why honesty exists in animal communication. All animals, humans included, engage in communication that involves both a sender and a receiver, and this communication occurs both within and across species. The sender communicates a signal to a receiver, who then acts upon that signal. Male peacocks seek to signal their worthiness to females of the species by growing vibrant tail feathers, and gazelles engage in spectacular leaping displays, called stotting, upon seeing approaching predators in order to indicate their athletic prowess and therefore dissuade the predator from attacking.

  In some cases, a signal may be honest, meaning that the sender is conveying accurate information, but signals can also be dishonest when the sender is providing false information to a receiver. All signals, however, share the additional feature of being costly to produce and send. A peacock’s tail feathers have significant weight and take excessive energy to produce. A bird chirping to alert others in the flock of a nearby predator has now exposed its own location to that same predator, potentially making it more vulnerable to attack.

  Biologists have found that, in general, animals communicate with honest signals, but why? Deception would seem to provide short-term gains. For example, the male fiddler crab is known for its one large fighting claw, which it uses to compete for a mate with other male crabs. If a fiddler crab loses its claw in the fight, another claw that is lighter in weight and therefore less effective grows in its place. Although the crab can still scare off other mates with its new claw that is similar in size to the original, it is sending the dishonest signal to other males that it is strong and able to fight, although if challenged it would likely lose. Scientists theorize that signals must be honest on average, at least to a certain degree. If not, the intended receiver would eventually evolve to ignore the signal, rendering it useless. In the early 1970s, biologist Amotz Zahavi proposed the handicap principle: honesty is maintained through handicaps, or high-cost signals, which are naturally more believable.

  The handicap principle relies on the assumption that prominent signals of fighting ability or selection may be impossible, or impossibly costly, to fake. For example, a full-grown bull elk’s rack of antlers may weigh in excess of 40 pounds, a weight greater than a young, weak, or sickly individual could hope to carry. Therefore, an elk able to grow a large rack of antlers is honestly signaling its ability to defend itself in a fight. Male peacock tail feathers present the same honest signal to females about the male’s strength and desirability as a mate. The long tail not only takes significant effort to produce, but also creates issues for survival. The bright colors are more attractive to predators, and the length of the tail somewhat restricts flight and therefore the bird’s ability to escape pursuit. As with the elk, a weaker or sicklier bird could not afford to produce such ornamentation. In both examples, the signal of strength is, in actuality, a handicap to the animal sending it.

  Thus, these revealing handicaps are honest signals of strength in the sense that some members of the species are better equipped to handle the costs of these handicaps. While most biologists agree on the power of handicaps to maintain honest signaling, the question still debated is that of choice. Are handicaps the result of a genetic condition that allows only certain members of the species to express the handicap fully? Or do they happen when a more capable individual actively chooses to take on a visible hardship and therefore signal to others its underlying strength or abilities?

  The condition-dependent model suggests that the level of display of a behavior or trait is directly proportional to genetic quality and environmental conditions and therefore cannot be faked. Some point to bright, iridescent plumage of some bird species as a condition-dependent, and therefore honest, handicap. Without limited physical wear, low parasite load, and a nutritious diet, these shiny feathers are impossible to maintain. The choice model, alternatively, was advocated in 1990 by biologist Alan Grafen, who claimed that all animals have the option to display a large handicap, but each must choose whether and to what extent to display that handicap, according to its knowledge of its own ability level. The optional signal remains honest because low-quality individuals will never signal at a level that is higher than would be advantageous for them, because of the increased cost of those signals. 

(P1)  There is a history of controversy within evolutionary biology over why honesty exists in animal communication. All animals, humans included, engage in communication that involves both a sender and a receiver, and this communication occurs both within and across species. The sender communicates a signal to a receiver, who then acts upon that signal. Male peacocks seek to signal their worthiness to females of the species by growing vibrant tail feathers, and gazelles engage in spectacular leaping displays, called stotting, upon seeing approaching predators in order to indicate their athletic prowess and therefore dissuade the predator from attacking.

Q:  According to paragraph 1, what is true of animal communication?
It often indicates the athletic prowess of the sender.
It involves a receiver who acts upon the communication sent.
Senders and receivers must be of the same species.
Evolutionary biologists agree on the reasons why it is honest.
(P2)  In some cases, a signal may be honest, meaning that the sender is conveying accurate information, but signals can also be dishonest when the sender is providing false information to a receiver. All signals, however, share the additional feature of being costly to produce and send. A peacock’s tail feathers have significant weight and take excessive energy to produce. A bird chirping to alert others in the flock of a nearby predator has now exposed its own location to that same predator, potentially making it more vulnerable to attack.

Q:  Why does the author mention “a bird chirping to alert others”?
To point out an example of a dishonest signal
To illustrate cross-species communication
To outline the requirements of an honest signal
To show how some signals are costly to send
(P2)  In some cases, a signal may be honest, meaning that the sender is conveying accurate information, but signals can also be dishonest when the sender is providing false information to a receiver. All signals, however, share the additional feature of being costly to produce and send. A peacock’s tail feathers have significant weight and take excessive energy to produce. A bird chirping to alert others in the flock of a nearby predator has now exposed its own location to that same predator, potentially making it more vulnerable to attack.

Q:  The word “conveying” in the passage is closest in meaning to
Transmitting
Interpreting
Withholding
Manufacturing
(P3)  Biologists have found that, in general, animals communicate with honest signals, but why? Deception would seem to provide short-term gains. For example, the male fiddler crab is known for its one large fighting claw, which it uses to compete for a mate with other male crabs. If a fiddler crab loses its claw in the fight, another claw that is lighter in weight and therefore less effective grows in its place. Although the crab can still scare off other mates with its new claw that is similar in size to the original, it is sending the dishonest signal to other males that it is strong and able to fight, although if challenged it would likely lose. Scientists theorize that signals must be honest on average, at least to a certain degree. If not, the intended receiver would eventually evolve to ignore the signal, rendering it useless. In the early 1970s, biologist Amotz Zahavi proposed the handicap principle: honesty is maintained through handicaps, or high-cost signals, which are naturally more believable.

Q:  According to paragraph 3, all of the following are characteristics of the replacement claw grown by a fiddler crab EXCEPT:
It is similar in size to the original claw.
It is not as heavy as the original claw.
It is no less effective than the original claw.
It functions as a dishonest signal.
→(P4)  The handicap principle relies on the assumption that prominent signals of fighting ability or selection may be impossible, or impossibly costly, to fake. For example, a full-grown bull elk’s rack of antlers may weigh in excess of 40 pounds, a weight greater than a young, weak, or sickly individual could hope to carry. Therefore, an elk able to grow a large rack of antlers is honestly signaling its ability to defend itself in a fight. Male peacock tail feathers present the same honest signal to females about the male’s strength and desirability as a mate. The long tail not only takes significant effort to produce, but also creates issues for survival. The bright colors are more attractive to predators, and the length of the tail somewhat restricts flight and therefore the bird’s ability to escape pursuit. As with the elk, a weaker or sicklier bird could not afford to produce such ornamentation. In both examples, the signal of strength is, in actuality, a handicap to the animal sending it.

Q:  According to paragraph 4, the antlers of a full-grown bull elk are useful because
Large, heavy antlers act as an authentic signal of strength
They allow scientists to estimate the age of the bull elk
They can be faked by sickly elks, which can then avoid costly fights
They restrict the elk’s ability to escape pursuit
→(P4)  The handicap principle relies on the assumption that prominent signals of fighting ability or selection may be impossible, or impossibly costly, to fake. For example, a full-grown bull elk’s rack of antlers may weigh in excess of 40 pounds, a weight greater than a young, weak, or sickly individual could hope to carry. Therefore, an elk able to grow a large rack of antlers is honestly signaling its ability to defend itself in a fight. Male peacock tail feathers present the same honest signal to females about the male’s strength and desirability as a mate. The long tail not only takes significant effort to produce, but also creates issues for survival. The bright colors are more attractive to predators, and the length of the tail somewhat restricts flight and therefore the bird’s ability to escape pursuit. As with the elk, a weaker or sicklier bird could not afford to produce such ornamentation. In both examples, the signal of strength is, in actuality, a handicap to the animal sending it.

Q:  The phrase “in actuality” in the passage is closest in meaning to
In theory
Intentionally
In reality
Unfortunately
→(P5)  Thus, these revealing handicaps are honest signals of strength in the sense that some members of the species are better equipped to handle the costs of these handicaps. While most biologists agree on the power of handicaps to maintain honest signaling, the question still debated is that of choice. Are handicaps the result of a genetic condition that allows only certain members of the species to express the handicap fully? Or do they happen when a more capable individual actively chooses to take on a visible hardship and therefore signal to others its underlying strength or abilities?

Q:  The word “equipped” in the passage is closest in meaning to
Exposed
Evolved
Prepared
Disarmed
→(P5)  Thus, these revealing handicaps are honest signals of strength in the sense that some members of the species are better equipped to handle the costs of these handicaps. While most biologists agree on the power of handicaps to maintain honest signaling, the question still debated is that of choice. Are handicaps the result of a genetic condition that allows only certain members of the species to express the handicap fully? Or do they happen when a more capable individual actively chooses to take on a visible hardship and therefore signal to others its underlying strength or abilities?

Q:  According to paragraph 5, biologists continue to argue over which of the following with regard to handicaps?
Whether handicaps are an effective way to maintain honest signaling
Whether some individuals are better able to handle the costs of handicaps
Whether all handicaps are honest signals
Whether capable individuals choose to take on handicaps
→(P5)  Thus, these revealing handicaps are honest signals of strength in the sense that some members of the species are better equipped to handle the costs of these handicaps. While most biologists agree on the power of handicaps to maintain honest signaling, the question still debated is that of choice. Are handicaps the result of a genetic condition that allows only certain members of the species to express the handicap fully? Or do they happen when a more capable individual actively chooses to take on a visible hardship and therefore signal to others its underlying strength or abilities?

Q:  The word “underlying” in the passage is closest in meaning to
Tremendous
Physical
Relative
Inherent
→(P6)  The condition-dependent model suggests that the level of display of a behavior or trait is directly proportional to genetic quality and environmental conditions and therefore cannot be faked. Some point to bright, iridescent plumage of some bird species as a condition-dependent, and therefore honest, handicap. Without limited physical wear, low parasite load, and a nutritious diet, these shiny feathers are impossible to maintain. The choice model, alternatively, was advocated in 1990 by biologist Alan Grafen, who claimed that all animals have the option to display a large handicap, but each must choose whether and to what extent to display that handicap, according to its knowledge of its own ability level. The optional signal remains honest because low-quality individuals will never signal at a level that is higher than would be advantageous for them, because of the increased cost of those signals. 

Q:  What can be inferred from paragraph 6 about certain birds with shiny, iridescent feathers?
They are choosing to maintain such feathers as a display.
They are honestly signaling their good health.
They are handicapped by low-nutrient diets.
It is a low-cost signal that can be feigned.
→(P6)  The condition-dependent model suggests that the level of display of a behavior or trait is directly proportional to genetic quality and environmental conditions and therefore cannot be faked. Some point to bright, iridescent plumage of some bird species as a condition-dependent, and therefore honest, handicap. Without limited physical wear, low parasite load, and a nutritious diet, these shiny feathers are impossible to maintain. The choice model, alternatively, was advocated in 1990 by biologist Alan Grafen, who claimed that all animals have the option to display a large handicap, but each must choose whether and to what extent to display that handicap, according to its knowledge of its own ability level. The optional signal remains honest because low-quality individuals will never signal at a level that is higher than would be advantageous for them, because of the increased cost of those signals. 

Q:  Which of the sentences below best expresses the essential information in the highlighted sentence in paragraph 3? Incorrect choices change the meaning in important ways or leave out essential information.
Because weaker individuals will not choose costly signals, such signals must be honest.
Higher-quality individuals are the only ones capable of choosing and producing honest signals.
Weaker individuals will not survive if they choose to display costly signals.
A chosen signal cannot be considered honest if it does not have an increased cost.
Whole Passage
 
P1 There is a history of controversy within evolutionary biology over why honesty exists in animal communication…
 
P2 In some cases, a signal may be honest, meaning that the sender is conveying accurate information, but signals can also be dishonest when the sender is providing false information to a receiver…
 
P3 Biologists have found that, in general, animals communicate with honest signals, but why?…

P4 The handicap principle relies on the assumption that prominent signals of fighting ability or selection may be impossible, or impossibly costly, to fake…
 
P5 Thus, these revealing handicaps are honest signals of strength in the sense that some members of the species are better equipped to handle the costs of these handicaps…

P6 The condition-dependent model suggests that the level of display of a behavior or trait is directly proportional to genetic quality and environmental conditions and therefore cannot be faked…

Q:  The passage mentions which of the following as a dishonest signal?
A male peacock’s long ornamental tail feathers
The stotting behavior of a gazelle
A bird chirping in response to a predator
A fiddler crab’s regrown fighting claw
In paragraph 3 of the passage, there is a missing sentence. The paragraph is repeated below and shows four letters [A], [B], [C], and [D] that indicate where the following sentence could be added.
 
This dishonest signal, however, appears to be the exception.
 
Where would the sentence best fit?

→(P3)   Biologists have found that, in general, animals communicate with honest signals, but why? Deception would seem to provide short-term gains.[A] For example, the male fiddler crab is known for its one large fighting claw, which it uses to compete for a mate with other male crabs.[B] If a fiddler crab loses its claw in the fight, another claw that is lighter in weight and therefore less effective grows in its place. Although the crab can still scare off other mates with its new claw that is similar in size to the original, it is sending the dishonest signal to other males that it is strong and able to fight, although if challenged it would likely lose. [C]Scientists theorize that signals must be honest on average, at least to a certain degree. If not, the intended receiver would eventually evolve to ignore the signal, rendering it useless.[D] In the early 1970s, biologist Amotz Zahavi proposed the handicap principle: honesty is maintained through handicaps, or high-cost signals, which are naturally more believable.
Directions: An introductory sentence for a brief summary of the passage is provided below. Complete the summary by selecting the THREE answer choices that express the most important ideas in the passage. Some sentences do not belong in the summary because they express ideas that are not presented in the passage or are minor ideas in the passage. This question is worth 2 points

 
  • Why animal signaling is typically honest is a question that many evolutionary biologists have tried to understand.
Because a weak individual is likely to have a less nutritious diet, its ability to produce a high-cost signal is reduced.
All animals communicate with one another, both within and across species, through signals that carry some cost to send.
It is unclear whether individuals choose to exhibit specific handicaps or whether only some individuals are genetically capable of so doing.
One typical purpose of signals that animals send to other members of their species is to indicate mating desirability.
According to the handicap principle, honest signaling dominates because high-cost signals are more believable.
By 1990, much of the debate over the issue of choice in animal signaling was resolved in favor of the condition-dependent model.
{"name":"TOEFL Experts Reading Practice 37", "url":"https://www.quiz-maker.com/QPREVIEW","txt":"Reading Section This section measures your ability to understand academic passages in English. You can skip questions and go back to them later as long as there is time remaining. Now begin the Reading section.","img":"https://cdn.poll-maker.com/29-1043861/01-male-bird-shutting-up-female-bird.jpg?sz=1200-00000009211000005300"}
Powered by: Quiz Maker