TOEFL Experts Reading Practice 34

 
Reading Section
 
 
This section measures your ability to understand academic passages in English. You can skip questions and go back to them later as long as there is time remaining.
 
 
 
 
Now begin the Reading section.
 
Reading Section
 
 
This section measures your ability to understand academic passages in English. You can skip questions and go back to them later as long as there is time remaining.
 
 
 
 
Now begin the Reading section.
Synesthesia
 
indir

  Does the sound of music have a color to you? Can you taste the sight of a particular shade of blue? Few people can. For most of us, the five primary senses—sight, hearing, taste, smell, and touch—are distinct categories; our brains process these sensory experiences as separate from one another and fundamentally unique. But for certain people known as “synesthetes,” things are not so straightforward. For these people, the stimulation of one sense may trigger the activation of one or more other senses.

  Just how prevalent synesthesia is remains unknown. Estimates range from 1 in 20 people to 1 in 25,000 people. The most common type of synesthesia is called “grapheme-color synesthesia,” or seeing different letters as different colors. Still, there are many people who experience other forms of overlapping senses. One such person was the American composer Leonard Bernstein, who reported that when he listened to music, the timbre1 of various sounds appeared to him visually as varied colors.

  Other synesthetes may even have multiple forms of the condition at once. Twentieth-century Russian journalist Solomon Shereshevsky, for example, is reputed to have had a spectacular memory—he could memorize a speech word for word after hearing it only once, and could remember a complicated math formula within a strikingly short time frame. The method by which he did so was synesthesia, which for him existed among all five senses.

  How does synesthesia work? In recent decades, scientists have been able to uncover the neurological basis for some kinds of synesthesia. Grapheme-color synesthetes, for example, display uncharacteristic neural activity in the color-based region of the visual cortex of their brains while reading, while people who are not grapheme-color synesthetes do not.

  The genetic basis of synesthesia, however, remains somewhat of a mystery. In the early days of genetic research into the phenomenon, scientists hypothesized that the gene that brought about synesthesia was a dominant one located on the X chromosome. This was believed in part because the phenomenon appeared to be more common among women than men.

  This belief has changed, however, as new research has challenged it. While there does appear to be a genetic basis of some sort, as revealed by the unusual brain activity associated with the condition, several developments have undermined the hypothesis that a dominant gene on the X chromosome is the precise genetic mechanism. One such development is that while early studies showed that more women than men are synesthetes, more recent and rigorous studies have suggested a more even distribution between the sexes. Furthermore, synesthesia can skip a generation, which means that if there is a gene for synesthesia, it cannot be a dominant gene, because dominant genes are not able to skip generations.

  A study of a pair of identical female twins, one of whom is synesthetic and one of whom is not, raised further questions about the genetic basis of the phenomenon. Identical twins have the same genetic code—all of their genes are identical. For one to be synesthetic and the other not to be, therefore, is peculiar. The study raised the possibility of whether synesthesia can be environmentally triggered or suppressed. At the very least, it suggested there might be some explanation beyond the simple inheritance or non-inheritance of a synesthesia-causing gene.

  But there is little evidence that synesthesia can be manipulated by one’s environment. Efforts to “train” people to be synesthetes have been unsuccessful compared to actual synesthetes. While people can “teach” their brains to correlate certain letters with certain colors, for instance, the neural activity in their brains remains distinct from that of people who are naturally synesthetic.

  A likely possibility is that the genetic basis for synesthesia does indeed lie on the X chromosome, as predicted by early researchers, and that the twin study can be explained by a phenomenon called “X-inactivation.” X-inactivation is the process by which one of a female’s two X chromosomes is rendered essentially inactive by the other X chromosome. X-inactivation occurs randomly and at the cellular level during the earliest stages of development of the female. In other words, one X chromosome may be active in one cell (while the other one is silenced), but in the neighboring cell, the situation is reversed. This genetic phenomenon could explain the twin study, because it leads to a different expression of the same genes in two female twins, which is precisely what was observed.

timbre1: the particular quality or tenor of sound or music, distinguishing, say, violins from trumpets playing the same note

Synesthesia
 
indir

  Does the sound of music have a color to you? Can you taste the sight of a particular shade of blue? Few people can. For most of us, the five primary senses—sight, hearing, taste, smell, and touch—are distinct categories; our brains process these sensory experiences as separate from one another and fundamentally unique. But for certain people known as “synesthetes,” things are not so straightforward. For these people, the stimulation of one sense may trigger the activation of one or more other senses.

  Just how prevalent synesthesia is remains unknown. Estimates range from 1 in 20 people to 1 in 25,000 people. The most common type of synesthesia is called “grapheme-color synesthesia,” or seeing different letters as different colors. Still, there are many people who experience other forms of overlapping senses. One such person was the American composer Leonard Bernstein, who reported that when he listened to music, the timbre1 of various sounds appeared to him visually as varied colors.

  Other synesthetes may even have multiple forms of the condition at once. Twentieth-century Russian journalist Solomon Shereshevsky, for example, is reputed to have had a spectacular memory—he could memorize a speech word for word after hearing it only once, and could remember a complicated math formula within a strikingly short time frame. The method by which he did so was synesthesia, which for him existed among all five senses.

  How does synesthesia work? In recent decades, scientists have been able to uncover the neurological basis for some kinds of synesthesia. Grapheme-color synesthetes, for example, display uncharacteristic neural activity in the color-based region of the visual cortex of their brains while reading, while people who are not grapheme-color synesthetes do not.

  The genetic basis of synesthesia, however, remains somewhat of a mystery. In the early days of genetic research into the phenomenon, scientists hypothesized that the gene that brought about synesthesia was a dominant one located on the X chromosome. This was believed in part because the phenomenon appeared to be more common among women than men.

  This belief has changed, however, as new research has challenged it. While there does appear to be a genetic basis of some sort, as revealed by the unusual brain activity associated with the condition, several developments have undermined the hypothesis that a dominant gene on the X chromosome is the precise genetic mechanism. One such development is that while early studies showed that more women than men are synesthetes, more recent and rigorous studies have suggested a more even distribution between the sexes. Furthermore, synesthesia can skip a generation, which means that if there is a gene for synesthesia, it cannot be a dominant gene, because dominant genes are not able to skip generations.

  A study of a pair of identical female twins, one of whom is synesthetic and one of whom is not, raised further questions about the genetic basis of the phenomenon. Identical twins have the same genetic code—all of their genes are identical. For one to be synesthetic and the other not to be, therefore, is peculiar. The study raised the possibility of whether synesthesia can be environmentally triggered or suppressed. At the very least, it suggested there might be some explanation beyond the simple inheritance or non-inheritance of a synesthesia-causing gene.

  But there is little evidence that synesthesia can be manipulated by one’s environment. Efforts to “train” people to be synesthetes have been unsuccessful compared to actual synesthetes. While people can “teach” their brains to correlate certain letters with certain colors, for instance, the neural activity in their brains remains distinct from that of people who are naturally synesthetic.

  A likely possibility is that the genetic basis for synesthesia does indeed lie on the X chromosome, as predicted by early researchers, and that the twin study can be explained by a phenomenon called “X-inactivation.” X-inactivation is the process by which one of a female’s two X chromosomes is rendered essentially inactive by the other X chromosome. X-inactivation occurs randomly and at the cellular level during the earliest stages of development of the female. In other words, one X chromosome may be active in one cell (while the other one is silenced), but in the neighboring cell, the situation is reversed. This genetic phenomenon could explain the twin study, because it leads to a different expression of the same genes in two female twins, which is precisely what was observed.

timbre1: the particular quality or tenor of sound or music, distinguishing, say, violins from trumpets playing the same note

(P1)  Does the sound of music have a color to you? Can you taste the sight of a particular shade of blue? Few people can. For most of us, the five primary senses—sight, hearing, taste, smell, and touch—are distinct categories; our brains process these sensory experiences as separate from one another and fundamentally unique. But for certain people known as “synesthetes,” things are not so straightforward. For these people, the stimulation of one sense may trigger the activation of one or more other senses.

Q:  The word shade in the passage is closest in meaning to
Hue
Glimpse
Shadow
Light
(P1)  Does the sound of music have a color to you? Can you taste the sight of a particular shade of blue? Few people can. For most of us, the five primary senses—sight, hearing, taste, smell, and touch—are distinct categories; our brains process these sensory experiences as separate from one another and fundamentally unique. But for certain people known as “synesthetes,” things are not so straightforward. For these people, the stimulation of one sense may trigger the activation of one or more other senses.

Q:  Which of the following can be inferred from paragraph 1?
The purpose of sensory perception is to separate sensory experiences into separate categories.
Generally, synesthetes process sensory experiences as unique and independent.
Sensory perception is substantially more difficult for synesthetes than for others.
Synesthetes do not always perceive different sensory experiences as separate and unique.
(P2)  Just how prevalent synesthesia is remains unknown. Estimates range from 1 in 20 people to 1 in 25,000 people. The most common type of synesthesia is called “grapheme-color synesthesia,” or seeing different letters as different colors. Still, there are many people who experience other forms of overlapping senses. One such person was the American composer Leonard Bernstein, who reported that when he listened to music, the timbre1 of various sounds appeared to him visually as varied colors.

Q:  The word “prevalent” in the passage is closest in meaning to
Successful
Common
Influential
Unfavorable
→(P2)  Just how prevalent synesthesia is remains unknown. Estimates range from 1 in 20 people to 1 in 25,000 people. The most common type of synesthesia is called “grapheme-color synesthesia,” or seeing different letters as different colors. Still, there are many people who experience other forms of overlapping senses. One such person was the American composer Leonard Bernstein, who reported that when he listened to music, the timbre1 of various sounds appeared to him visually as varied colors.

Q:  According to paragraph 2, what kind of synesthesia did Leonard Bernstein experience?
When the colors in his field of vision changed, he heard different sounds.
Different qualities of sounds appeared to him as various colors.
The louder the music he heard was, the more colorful his vision became.
It was impossible for him to hear music without seeing letters.
(P3)  Other synesthetes may even have multiple forms of the condition at once. Twentieth-century Russian journalist Solomon Shereshevsky, for example, is reputed to have had a spectacular memory—he could memorize a speech word for word after hearing it only once, and could remember a complicated math formula within a strikingly short time frame. The method by which he did so was synesthesia, which for him existed among all five senses.

(P4)  How does synesthesia work? In recent decades, scientists have been able to uncover the neurological basis for some kinds of synesthesia. Grapheme-color synesthetes, for example, display uncharacteristic neural activity in the color-based region of the visual cortex of their brains while reading, while people who are not grapheme-color synesthetes do not.


Q:  According to paragraph 3, Solomon Shereshevsky had a remarkable memory because of which of the following?
He could quickly recall complex math formulas.
He primarily used vivid colors to remember.
His memories involved several senses at once.
He worked as a Russian journalist in the twentieth century.
(P3)  Other synesthetes may even have multiple forms of the condition at once. Twentieth-century Russian journalist Solomon Shereshevsky, for example, is reputed to have had a spectacular memory—he could memorize a speech word for word after hearing it only once, and could remember a complicated math formula within a strikingly short time frame. The method by which he did so was synesthesia, which for him existed among all five senses.

(P4)  How does synesthesia work? In recent decades, scientists have been able to uncover the neurological basis for some kinds of synesthesia. Grapheme-color synesthetes, for example, display uncharacteristic neural activity in the color-based region of the visual cortex of their brains while reading, while people who are not grapheme-color synesthetes do not.


Q:  Which of the sentences below best expresses the essential information in the highlighted portion of the passage? Incorrect choices change the meaning in important ways or leave out essential information.
When grapheme-color synesthetes read, a region of their brains related to perceiving color are unusually active.
Grapheme-color synesthetes read when their brains display unusual activity in a color-based region of the visual cortex.
While reading text in various colors, grapheme-color synesthetes show unusual activity in the visual cortex of their brains.
The brains of grapheme-color synesthetes are unusually active in the color-based region of the visual cortex.
→(P5)  The genetic basis of synesthesia, however, remains somewhat of a mystery. In the early days of genetic research into the phenomenon, scientists hypothesized that the gene that brought about synesthesia was a dominant one located on the X chromosome. This was believed in part because the phenomenon appeared to be more common among women than men.

→(P6)  This belief has changed, however, as new research has challenged it. While there does appear to be a genetic basis of some sort, as revealed by the unusual brain activity associated with the condition, several developments have undermined the hypothesis that a dominant gene on the X chromosome is the precise genetic mechanism. One such development is that while early studies showed that more women than men are synesthetes, more recent and rigorous studies have suggested a more even distribution between the sexes. Furthermore, synesthesia can skip a generation, which means that if there is a gene for synesthesia, it cannot be a dominant gene, because dominant genes are not able to skip generations.


Q:  Which of the following can be inferred from paragraphs 5 and 6 about the studies showing synesthesia to be more common among women than men?
They have undermined an early hypothesis about the genetic basis of synesthesia.
They were correct at the time, but recent population changes have made them irrelevant.
They seem to have not been as accurate as later studies investigating the same issue.
They superseded earlier studies showing synesthesia to be evenly distributed between the sexes.
→(P5)  The genetic basis of synesthesia, however, remains somewhat of a mystery. In the early days of genetic research into the phenomenon, scientists hypothesized that the gene that brought about synesthesia was a dominant one located on the X chromosome. This was believed in part because the phenomenon appeared to be more common among women than men.

→(P6)  This belief has changed, however, as new research has challenged it. While there does appear to be a genetic basis of some sort, as revealed by the unusual brain activity associated with the condition, several developments have undermined the hypothesis that a dominant gene on the X chromosome is the precise genetic mechanism. One such development is that while early studies showed that more women than men are synesthetes, more recent and rigorous studies have suggested a more even distribution between the sexes. Furthermore, synesthesia can skip a generation, which means that if there is a gene for synesthesia, it cannot be a dominant gene, because dominant genes are not able to skip generations.


Q:  According to paragraph 5, synesthesia was once believed by researchers to be caused by
A gene situated on the non-dominant X chromosome
A non-dominant gene present only in women’s chromosomes
A dominant gene that was situated on the X chromosome
A dominant gene not located on the X chromosome
→(P5)  The genetic basis of synesthesia, however, remains somewhat of a mystery. In the early days of genetic research into the phenomenon, scientists hypothesized that the gene that brought about synesthesia was a dominant one located on the X chromosome. This was believed in part because the phenomenon appeared to be more common among women than men.

→(P6)  This belief has changed, however, as new research has challenged it. While there does appear to be a genetic basis of some sort, as revealed by the unusual brain activity associated with the condition, several developments have undermined the hypothesis that a dominant gene on the X chromosome is the precise genetic mechanism. One such development is that while early studies showed that more women than men are synesthetes, more recent and rigorous studies have suggested a more even distribution between the sexes. Furthermore, synesthesia can skip a generation, which means that if there is a gene for synesthesia, it cannot be a dominant gene, because dominant genes are not able to skip generations.


Q:  According to paragraph 6, which of the following is true in general of genes that are dominant?
They are not located on the X chromosome.
They form the genetic basis of conditions such as synesthesia.
They are more evenly distributed between the sexes than previously thought.
They do not skip generations.
→(P7)  A study of a pair of identical female twins, one of whom is synesthetic and one of whom is not, raised further questions about the genetic basis of the phenomenon. Identical twins have the same genetic code—all of their genes are identical. For one to be synesthetic and the other not to be, therefore, is peculiar. The study raised the possibility of whether synesthesia can be environmentally triggered or suppressed. At the very least, it suggested there might be some explanation beyond the simple inheritance or non-inheritance of a synesthesia-causing gene.

Q:  According to paragraph 7, the study of the two identical twins provided reason to believe which of the following?
Environmental factors may be able to cause or at least trigger synesthesia.
It is unlikely that synesthesia is influenced by genetic conditions.
The non-synesthetic twin lacked the gene for synesthesia that the other twin had.
Claims of synesthetic experiences by just one twin were probably mistaken.
→(P8)  But there is little evidence that synesthesia can be manipulated by one’s environment. Efforts to “train” people to be synesthetes have been unsuccessful compared to actual synesthetes. While people can “teach” their brains to correlate certain letters with certain colors, for instance, the neural activity in their brains remains distinct from that of people who are naturally synesthetic.

Q:  TThe word “correlate” in the passage is closest in meaning to
Present
Provide
Repress
Associate
In paragraph 9 of the passage, there is a missing sentence. The paragraph is repeated below and shows four letters [A], [B], [C], and [D] that indicate where the following sentence could be added.
 
In contrast, X-inactivation is not experienced by males, who have only one X chromosome.

Where would the sentence best fit?

→(P9)  A likely possibility is that the genetic basis for synesthesia does indeed lie on the X chromosome, as predicted by early researchers, and that the twin study can be explained by a phenomenon called “X-inactivation.” [A] X-inactivation is the process by which one of a female’s two X chromosomes is rendered essentially inactive by the other X chromosome.[B] X-inactivation occurs randomly and at the cellular level during the earliest stages of development of the female. In other words, one X chromosome may be active in one cell (while the other one is silenced), but in the neighboring cell, the situation is reversed.[C] This genetic phenomenon could explain the twin study, because it leads to a different expression of the same genes in two female twins, which is precisely what was observed.[D]
Which of the following best describes the author’s presentation of information in the passage?
A number of studies are presented to support the assertion that synesthesia is primarily an environmental phenomenon.
The author uses logic to show that doubt about the causes of synesthesia is unwarranted.
The author introduces the condition of synesthesia and explores possible causes.
Two opposing points of view about synesthesia are described; the author concludes by advocating for further research.
Directions: An introductory sentence for a brief summary of the passage is provided below. Complete the summary by selecting the THREE answer choices that express the most important ideas in the passage. Some sentences do not belong in the summary because they express ideas that are not presented in the passage or are minor ideas in the passage. This question is worth 2 points

 
  • Synesthesia is a neurological phenomenon by which a small percentage of people have blended sensory experiences.

Great musicians and geniuses are often synesthetes, perceiving their medium in ways non-synesthetes cannot.
Synesthesia manifests itself in various forms and can involve two or more senses at once.
The true prevalence of synesthesia is likely to continue to be unknown.
Research suggests that synesthesia may be linked to a gene located on the X chromosome.
The neurological activity of synesthetes is unusual, in a way that cannot be simulated, it seems.
As inherited genetic material, dominant genes are unable to skip generations.
{"name":"TOEFL Experts Reading Practice 34", "url":"https://www.quiz-maker.com/QPREVIEW","txt":"Reading Section This section measures your ability to understand academic passages in English. You can skip questions and go back to them later as long as there is time remaining. Now begin the Reading section.","img":"https://cdn.poll-maker.com/29-1035382/tmp885557389095337986.jpg?sz=1200-00000000000790005300"}
Powered by: Quiz Maker